Skip to main content

A genetic association analysis of cognitive ability and cognitive ageing using 325 markers for 109 genes associated with oxidative stress or cognition

Abstract

Background

Non-pathological cognitive ageing is a distressing condition affecting an increasing number of people in our 'ageing society'. Oxidative stress is hypothesised to have a major role in cellular ageing, including brain ageing.

Results

Associations between cognitive ageing and 325 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), located in 109 genes implicated in oxidative stress and/or cognition, were examined in a unique cohort of relatively healthy older people, on whom we have cognitive ability scores at ages 11 and 79 years (LBC1921). SNPs showing a significant positive association were then genotyped in a second cohort for whom we have cognitive ability scores at the ages of 11 and 64 years (ABC1936). An intronic SNP in the APP gene (rs2830102) was significantly associated with cognitive ageing in both LBC1921 and a combined LBC1921/ABC1936 analysis (p < 0.01), but not in ABC1936 alone.

Conclusion

This study suggests a possible role for APP in normal cognitive ageing, in addition to its role in Alzheimer's disease.

Background

Individuals differ in their cognitive skills, and in how much these cognitive skills change as people grow older. That is, there are individual differences in the trait (or level) of intelligence, and in the age-related change (or trajectory). We have previously shown that about 50% of the variance in trait intelligence is stable from the age of 11 to the age of 79[1]. In both the trait and the age-related change, the majority of the between-individual variation is accounted for by a common factor of general cognitive ability (or g)[2, 3]. Both mild intellectual impairment (low trait intelligence) and accelerated age-related cognitive decline (increased downward trajectory in intelligence) have a major impact on society, because of the large number of individuals involved who have limited independence. In our increasingly 'ageing society', disabilities linked to cognitive ageing are a growing medical and social problem.

There are environmental and genetic contributions to individual differences in trait intelligence and cognitive ageing[4, 5]. Genetic influences account for more than 50% of the variability in adult cognitive abilities[6]. We have shown that genetic variation in some specific genes, e.g. APOE is associated with change in cognitive ability with age, but not with the stable trait of intelligence[7]. Therefore, it is likely that some genetic variants are associated with life-long cognitive abilities and others specifically with variance in age-related cognitive decline. The search for genetic contributions to cognitive ageing can be guided by focussing on mechanisms that affect brain ageing[5].

Oxidative stress is hypothesised to be a significant contributor to cellular ageing. The free radical theory of ageing predicts that, with increasing age, free radicals, reactive by-products of oxidative metabolism, damage macromolecules such as DNA, protein and lipids[8, 9]. Support for the free radical hypothesis of ageing comes from a wide variety of sources, including analyses of mutations and transgenic animals (for recent reviews see[10, 11]). The brain is particularly vulnerable to oxidative damage as a result of its high aerobic metabolism and high concentrations of polyunsaturated fatty acids that are susceptible to lipid peroxidation [12–15].

Oxidative damage to mitochondrial DNA accumulates at a ten-fold higher rate than nuclear DNA, although its precise significance to ageing remains controversial[16, 17]. The constant leak of reactive oxygen species from mitochondria increases with age, and deficiency of both mitochondrial and cytoplasmic superoxide dismutase are associated with neurodegeneration due to oxidative damage [18–20]. A role for oxidative stress has been proposed both in Alzheimer's disease (AD), associated with amyloid plaques[21, 22], and in Parkinson's disease, with the presence of iron and auto-oxidised monoamines[23]. A role for oxidative stress has also been proposed in mild cognitive impairment[24, 25]. Non-pathological cognitive ageing was found to be related to differences in oxidative stress (measured, for example, by thiobarbituric acid reactive substances) in a large community study of older people[26]. It is also implicated in the "common cause hypothesis of ageing": the recent finding that physical and cognitive capabilities are highly correlated in old age[27].

Expression profiling of large gene arrays in adult and aged mouse brain also supports a role for oxidative damage in cognitive ageing[28, 29]. Lee et al[28] examined the expression profiles in neocortex and cerebellum of 6,347 genes in adult (5 months) and aged (30 months) mice. In both brain regions, gene expression profiles showed increased inflammatory response and oxidative stress gene expression in the older mice. These authors concluded that oxidative stress is an important and perhaps underlying cause of the ageing process in post-mitotic (neural) tissues. In a similar study, Jiang et al[29] probed over 11,000 genes in cortex and hypothalamus in 2 month and 22 month old mice and found altered expression for 98 genes (0.9%) in cortex, about 20% of which were also altered in hypothalamus. Significant changes (at least two-fold) were found in a variety of proteins, including eight concerned with oxidative stress response.

We previously identified associations between common functional polymorphisms in genes involved in AD or oxidative stress and cognitive ageing[7, 30, 31]. However, these studies all involved genotyping small numbers of polymorphisms in a small sample of genes. Technology is now available to genotype easily much larger numbers of polymorphisms. The aim of the present study was to investigate the influence of genetic variation in genes primarily related to oxidative stress and antioxidant defences in two cohorts of relatively healthy older individuals. These are the Lothian Birth Cohort of 1921 (LBC1921) and the Aberdeen Birth Cohort of 1936 (ABC1936), on whom cognitive ability test scores are available at age 11 and in later life; that is, they have data on the lifetime trait of intelligence, and lifetime cognitive change[32]. These cohorts form a unique resource to test for genes associated with cognitive ageing. Both cohorts took an identical mental ability test at age 11 and a different but overlapping series of cognitive ability tests at either age 79 (LBC1921) or age 64 (ABC1936)[32]. To utilise this resource a candidate gene genetic association study was performed by genotyping 387 SNPs in 444 members of LBC1921. We have ~80% power to detect an effect size of 3% at a type-1 error rate of 0.01. Replication of possible associations is important. Therefore, SNPs that showed a positive association with either cognitive ability at age 11 or cognitive ageing were then genotyped in 485 members of ABC1936.

Results

384 SNPs were selected for genotyping by the GoldenGate™ assay. A multiplex assay was successfully designed for 322 SNPs (83.9%). 437 (261 women, 176 men) of the 444 LBC1921 subjects (98.4%) were successfully genotyped for at least 316 SNPs. Genotyping data were obtained, from both samples, for 15 of the 16 subjects who were genotyped in duplicate and no discrepancies were identified. Three further SNPs were genotyped by TaqMan® technology in 424–434 of the subjects. In summary 325 SNPs were genotyped in 420–437 subjects. 86 SNPs (26.5%) were monomorphic in LBC1921.

LBC1921

Childhood cognitive ability in LBC1921

There was a nominally significant association between three SNPs and age 11 Moray House Test (MHT) score: CTSZ, rs9760 (F = 5.625, p = 0.004, η2 = 0.025); GSTZ1, rs3177429 (F = 4.820, p = 0.009, η2 = 0.022); NDUFS4 rs31304 (F = 9.757, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.022). The genotype frequencies for each of these SNPs did not differ significantly from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium.

Cognitive ageing in LBC1921

Table 1 indicates the effect of each polymorphic SNP (p-value) on each of the age 79 cognitive outcomes controlling for age 11 MHT score (i.e. the effect on cognitive ageing). Sex was included as a between subjects variable, except in the case of PRDX4 SNP rs552105 which is on the X chromosome. For this SNP men and women were analysed separately. Nine SNPs located in eight genes (APP, GLRX, HSPA9B, MSRB2, NDUFS1, NDUFV2, NDUFV3 and NOS1) showed a nominally significant association (p < 0.01) with one of the cognitive variables (table 2). The two SNPs in NDUFV3 were in almost complete linkage disequilibrium. Therefore, only rs8128440 was taken forward to the next stage. The minor allele frequency of SNP rs9658446 in NOS1 was only 4.58 × 10-3, and therefore this SNP was not carried forward to the next stage. The genotype frequencies for each of these SNPs did not differ significantly from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium.

Table 1 Effect of each polymorphic SNP on each of the cognitive outcomes, controlling for sex and age 11 cognitive ability.
Table 2 SNPs showing a significant (p < 0.01) association with at least one cognitive trait at age 79 (LBC1921), controlling for sex and age 11 cognitive ability.

Cognitive ability and ageing in ABC1936

Nine of the 10 SNPs that showed a positive association in LBC1921 with either age 11 cognitive ability or cognitive ageing were successfully genotyped in ABC1936 by KBiosciences. The APP SNP rs2830102 was genotyped using TaqMan® technology. None of the SNPs were significantly associated with either age 11 MHT score or cognitive ageing in ABC1936 (p > 0.01). Table 3 shows the effect of SNPs showing a positive association with at least one cognitive trait at age 79 (LBC1921), controlling for sex and age 11 cognitive ability, on cognitive traits at age 64 (ABC1936), controlling for sex and age 11 cognitive ability. The genotype frequencies for each of these SNPs did not differ significantly from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium.

Table 3 Effect of SNPs showing a significant (p < 0.01) association with at least one cognitive trait at age 79 (LBC1921), controlling for sex and age 11 cognitive ability, on cognitive traits at age 64 (ABC1936), controlling for sex and age 11 cognitive ability.

A combined LBC1921/ABC1936 analysis to detect associations with cognitive ageing

Because larger sample sizes have greater power to detect associations, general linear modelling was performed using combined data from LBC1921 and ABC1936 to investigate the effect of the seven SNPs that showed a significant association with cognitive ageing in LBC1921, on a relatively large sample size (n = 858–886). An effect size of just 2% can be detected with > 80% power at a type-1 error rate of 0.01 using 858 subjects. The effect size of any single polymorphism influencing variation in a complex trait like cognitive ageing may well be relatively small, as many polymorphisms are likely to be involved[5]. A combined LBC1921/ABC1936 univariate analysis was performed for the each of these seven SNP genotypes, with later life Raven's Progressive Matrices score (the only later life cognitive test that was measured in both cohorts) as the dependent variable. All the cognitive tests used to assess LBC1921 are significantly positively correlated[33] and, therefore, associations that were previously identified with tests other than Raven score may be detected with this test when using a larger sample size. Other effects included in the model were age 11 MHT score, sex and cohort (table 4). All interactions were non-significant and removed from the models. As previously shown[31] cohort and sex were significant for all SNP models (p < 0.001), with ABC1936 and males scoring higher than LBC1921 and females. Age 11 MHT score contributed significantly to later life Raven score (p < 0.001). This latter finding reflects the highly significant partial correlation between age 11 MHT score and later life Raven score, controlling for cohort (r = 0.52, df = 892, p < 0.001). APP intronic SNP, rs2830102, was significantly associated with later life Raven score, controlling for age 11 MHT score, sex and cohort (F = 5.988, p = 0.003, η2 = 0.014). Figure 1 shows the Raven score raw data (A), and the estimated marginal means (B), for later life Raven scores by sex and cohort, controlling for age 11 MHT score. G/G (genotype B/B in tables 2, 3 and 4) homozygotes scored significantly lower than both heterozygotes (p = 0.029) and A/A (genotype A/A in tables 2, 3 and 4) homozygotes (p = 0.002). There was a trend for heterozygotes to score lower than A/A homozygotes (p = 0.057). None of the other SNP genotypes were significantly associated with later life Raven score, controlling for age 11 MHT, sex and cohort (p > 0.01).

Table 4 Effect of SNPs showing a significant (p < 0.01) association with at least one cognitive trait at age 79 (LBC1921), controlling for sex and age 11 cognitive ability, on Raven's Progressive Matrices Score in later life controlling for sex, age 11 cognitive ability and cohort (ABC1936 or LBC1921).
Figure 1
figure 1

Score on Raven's Matrices by APP rs2830102 genotype, sex and cohort (LBC1921 or ABC1936): A) raw data; B) estimated marginal means from general linear model, adjusted for age 11 MHT score.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first large-scale investigation into the possible genetic contributions to the normal variability in cognitive ageing experienced by individuals. We examined genes previously implicated in oxidative stress, dementia and cognitive function. Of 325 gene variants analysed, nine were positively associated with variation in performance on one of four tests of cognitive ability at age 79 (LBC1921), controlling for sex and childhood cognitive ability. Two of these SNPs were in strong linkage disequilibrium and one SNP had a very low minor allele frequency. None of these associations was replicated in a second cohort of 64 year olds (ABC1936) who took a different but overlapping series of cognitive tests. Therefore, the present study should be considered as an informative, null study concerning a coherent set of genes that might have, but do not, affect normal cognitive ageing, beyond the effect size which it was powered to detect.

However, APP intronic SNP rs2830102 genotype was associated with non-verbal reasoning, as measured by Raven's Progressive Matrices, in a joint analysis of LBC1921 and ABC1936 data. It is emphasised, though, that it did not have significant effects in both cohorts separately, and as such it needs replicating in other cohorts. APP genotype accounted for 1.4% of the variance in the Raven scores, after adjustment for sex, cohort and childhood ability differences. Although this is a relatively small effect size, it is what is expected with a complex trait like cognitive ageing, where many variants are likely to be involved. APOE, which is one of the few genes that has been associated with cognitive ageing in several cohorts, including LBC1921[7], has an effect size of just 1% to 2% and is considered to be important. 12.4% of the variance was accounted for by the cohort of the participants. ABC1936 participants who, at age 64, were 15 years younger, scored significantly better than the 79-year-olds in the LBC1921 (p < 0.001). No significant interaction between year of cohort and APP genotype was identified.

APP encodes the amyloid β (Aβ) precursor protein. Extracellular Aβ plaques, which form in the meningeal vessels of AD patient brains, are a defining feature of the disease. Mutations in both the coding region[34] and the promoter region[35] of this gene have been associated with AD. Aberrant expression of APP has also been implicated in AD[36, 37]. AD is characterised by an impairment of multiple cognitive domains. Amyloidogenic peptide derivatives of mutant APP have been implicated in the generation of free radicals and with mitochondrial oxidative damage (reviewed in[38]). It is possible that common variation in APP DNA sequence is associated with variation in oxidative stress in the general population, leading to variation in normal cognitive ageing. This may reflect the possibility that the neurobiology of both cognitive ageing and AD is, to some extent, a continuum. It is also possible that the association between APP SNP rs2830102 and normal cognitive ageing, as measured using Raven's Progressive Matrices, is related to incipient AD in some members of LBC1921 and ABC1936.

SNP rs2830102 is located in intron 1 of APP and may affect regulation of gene expression. Although the SNP does not lie in a predicted promoter region[39] and is not predicted to alter splicing it does occur within a region of sequence conservation[40]. Alternatively it may be in linkage disequilibrium with another functional SNP, possibly in the promoter of the gene. It is important that the APP gene is investigated further for its role in both non-pathological cognitive ageing and AD.

Several of the SNPs investigated in this study had previously been associated with intelligence or AD (table 1). We failed to find any significant association between these SNPs and either cognitive ability at age 11 or cognitive ageing in LBC1921. Such attempted replications are important, because initial reports of genotype-phenotype associations often do not replicate.

To investigate genetic influences on non-pathological cognitive ageing we chose to perform a relatively large scale genetic association study using candidate genes, for which there was strong a priori evidence for their involvement in brain ageing. We focussed on a specific ageing-related mechanism, that of oxidative stress. We were in the invaluable position of being able to test directly for cognitive ageing across a long period of time, as we had cognitive ability scores at both age 11 and in later life. There has been much discussion in the literature regarding larger scale association study designs. We chose a candidate genes approach that allowed the use of smaller numbers of SNPs compared to a whole genome association study. However, it is likely that important regions of the genome were missed by this approach. We followed recent guidelines from a genomewide association scan workshop[41] that concluded that multistage designs, whereby a sub-set of subjects are initially genotyped and additional subjects are then genotyped for SNPs that show a positive association, enhanced the efficiency of such studies. We chose to genotype a limited number of potentially functional SNPs in a larger number of genes rather than to attempt to fully cover a smaller number of genes using, for example, tagging SNPs and may therefore have missed important SNPs whose functionality was not predicted. We considered this a more efficient use of limited genotyping funds. It allowed us to cover more of our candidate genes and increased the likelihood that we would identify a causative SNP, particularly as concern exists over the portability of tagging SNPs across populations. A few recent preliminary studies indicate that it may be possible to use tagging SNPs designed in one population to investigate associations in a second population, but this should only be done with caution [42–45]. With regard to the analysis, we decided to initially concentrate on the identification of individual SNPs that have a detectable main effect on variation in cognitive ageing. However, in the future we may include newly developed statistical techniques that allow the identification of interlocus interactions[46].

Like all large scale genetic association studies, this study suffers from the problem of multiple testing; we initially investigated 325 SNPs and four cognitive tests in 437 subjects. Because many of the SNPs are in linkage disequilibrium and, moreover, scores on the cognitive tests are positively correlated, it was deemed inappropriate to perform a Bonferroni-type correction. However, we were able to genotype SNPs showing a nominally significant association in the first cohort, with a second equally large and valuable cohort and, we used a relatively stringent p valueof < 0.01.

It is also important, given the relatively small size and younger age of the replication cohort (n = 485), that SNPs that showed a positive association in LBC1921, but not in ABC1936 or the combined cohorts, are investigated in future association studies to identify genetic determinants of cognitive ageing. A further caveat of the study is that ABC1936 did not take exactly the same cognitive tests as LBC1921. Therefore, associations identified in LBC1921 may have been with specific cognitive abilities that were not examined in ABC1936.

Conclusion

This study has identified a number of genes, for which there was strong a priori evidence for their involvement in cognitive ageing, which have an association with cognitive ageing in a cohort of relatively healthy 79 year old subjects (LBC1921). A significant association with a SNP in the gene encoding APP was also identified in a combined analysis of LBC1921 and a second younger cohort (ABC1936), suggesting its importance in cognitive ageing as well as AD. It is important that the role of this gene in cognitive ageing is investigated further.

Methods

Subjects

The subjects recruited to this study originally participated, at the age of about 11 years, in the Scottish Mental Surveys of either 1932 or 1947[32, 47, 48]. On June 1st 1932 and June 4th 1947 a valid mental ability test, a version of the Moray House Test No. 12 (MHT), was given to almost all Scottish children attending school on the Survey day who were born in 1921 (N = 87,498) or 1936 (N = 70,805), respectively.

Lothian Birth Cohort 1921 (LBC1921)

LBC1921 are surviving participants of the Scottish Mental Survey of 1932, who were living independently in the Edinburgh area at the time of recruitment. Further testing and recruitment details have been published previously[32]. Mean age at re-test was 79.1 years (SD = 0.6 years), and all subjects were Caucasian. The following inclusion criteria were applied: Cognitive ability scores were available at age 11 and age 79; there was no history of dementia; Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score was 24 or greater; and SNP genotyping was successful. This gave a total of 437 subjects (261 women, 176 men).

Aberdeen Birth Cohort 1936 (ABC1936)

ABC1936 are surviving participants of the Scottish Mental Survey of 1947, who were living independently in the city of Aberdeen at the time of recruitment. Further recruitment details have been published previously[49, 50]. Mean age at re-test was 64.6 years (SD = 0.7 years), and all subjects were Caucasian. The following inclusion criteria were applied: Cognitive ability scores were available at age 11 and age 64 and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score was 24 or greater. This gave a total of 485 subjects (246 women, 239 men).

Cognitive testing

Moray House Test No. 1(MHT)

All subjects took this general mental ability test at age 11, in the Scottish Mental Surveys of 1932 and 1947. LBC1921 re-took the test at about age 79. The test is described fully elsewhere[1, 32, 47]. The same instructions and the time limit (45 minutes) were used on both occasions. At re-test, ABC1936 took subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised instead of the MHT[51]: the Block Design, which measures visuo-spatial ability, and Digit Symbol, which measures speed of information processing[51].

Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)

MMSE[52] was used to screen both cohorts for possible dementia. Maximum score is 30. A score of less than 24 was used here as an exclusion criterion because it is often adopted as an indicator of possible dementia.

Both cohorts underwent a series of mental tests designed to examine different cognitive functions: non-verbal reasoning, executive function, and memory and learning. We have previously described this testing in detail[32, 53]. The individual cognitive functions of the two independent cohorts (LBC1921 and ABC1936) were examined using a different series of tests as indicated below:

Non-verbal reasoning

Raven's Progressive Matrices[54]

Non-verbal reasoning was examined in all subjects using Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices. The time limit was 20 minutes.

Executive Function

Verbal fluency

LBC1921 took the verbal fluency test, which is described as a test of prefrontal executive function[55, 56].

Uses of Common Objects

ABC1936 took the use of common objects test, which is described as a test of executive function or purposive action[55].

Verbal Memory and Learning

Logical Memory

LBC1921 took the Logical Memory test, which is a verbal declarative memory sub-test from Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised[57].

Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test

ABC1936 took the auditory verbal learning test which assesses short and longer term memory and learning[55].

Illumina SNP selection

A list of 141 brain-expressed genes was selected and provided to Illumina (table 5). They were selected if they were: a) implicated in antioxidant defence; b) vitagenes (longevity assurance processes); c) associated with cognitive function; d) associated with AD; e) "stress response" genes showing an increased expression in the aged mouse [28]; and/or f) nuclear genes encoding mitochondrial complex 1 proteins. From an initial list of 14,033 potential SNPs, 384 were selected for genotyping using the following criteria: a) all designable (including designability score 0.5) SNPs previously associated with AD and cognitive function; b) all designable (including designability score 0.5) functional SNPs; c) all non-synonymous validated and designable (including designability score 0.5) SNPs; d) all validated and designable (including designability score 0.5) SNPs at exon/intron boundaries that potentially alter splicing; e) all validated and designable (including designability score 0.5) SNPs with percentage identity in mouse >= 80%; f) all validated and designable (excluding designability score 0.5) SNPs with percentage identity in mouse between 60% and 80%; g) remaining SNPs were Illumina validated synonymous SNPs in previously unrepresented genes (see additional file 1). Designability is ranked as 0, 0.5 or 1. A "0" is assigned to SNPs for which an assay cannot be designed, "0.5" indicates the SNP has a designability score low enough to suggest that there might be challenges to the design, and "1" is reserved for those that do not appear to have any challenges in their designability. Validation class is ranked as 1, 2, or 3. "1" means that a SNP is nonvalidated, "2" is a two-hit SNP (non Illumina validated, i.e. it has been validated on some other platform on more than one chromosome), and "3" means two-hit Illumina validated. The percentage identity with mouse is based on a 120 base pair window surrounding the SNP.

Table 5 Cognitive Ageing Candidate Genes (expressed in the brain).

Genotyping of LBC1921

Genomic DNA was extracted from blood using standard methods. Genotyping of 384 SNPs was performed using the GoldenGate™ assay by the Illumina BeadLab service facility in San Diego. 444 LBC1921 subjects were genotyped, 16 of them in duplicate. A further three SNPs (MPO, rs7208693; TF, rs3811656 and NDUFAF1 rs3204853) were genotyped at the Welcome Trust Clinical Research Facility Genetics Core, Western General Hospital, Edinburgh[58] using TaqMan® technology (Applied Biosystems).

Genotyping of ABC1936

Genomic DNA was extracted from blood using standard methods. Genotyping in LBC1921 found seven independent SNPs significantly associated with cognitive ageing (p < 0.01), and three SNPs significantly associated with age 11 MHT score. Genotyping for these SNPs was attempted in ABC1936, using KASPar, by Kbiosciences (Herts, UK). In cases where a KBiosciences assay could not be designed, genotyping was performed at the Welcome Trust Clinical Research Facility Genetics Core, Western General Hospital, Edinburgh[58] using TaqMan® technology.

Statistical analysis

The power to detect a causative variant at a type-1 error rate of 0.01, for a variant explaining 2–3% of the variance, was estimated by calculating the non-centrality parameter of a non-central χ2 and the probability that the test statistic under the alternative hypothesis would be larger than the threshold corresponding to the specified type-1 error[59].

The effect of each SNP genotype on LBC1921 age 11 MHT score was analysed using general linear modelling (univariate analysis of variance). The fixed effects (between subjects variables) were: SNP genotype and sex.

The effect of each SNP genotype on each of the four age 79 cognitive outcome variables, for LBC1921, was analysed using general linear modelling (multivariate analysis of variance). The fixed effects were: SNP genotype and sex. Age 11 MHT score was included as a covariate, allowing us to identify associations specifically with cognitive ageing.

General linear modelling, as described above, was used to identify associations between SNPs that showed a positive association in LBC1921 (with either age 11 MHT score or cognitive ageing), and age 11 MHT score and each of the five age 64 cognitive outcome variables (controlling for age 11 MHT score), for ABC1936.

The raw data from LBC1921 and ABC1936 were combined and the effect of each SNP on the Raven's Progressive Matrices Score was analysed using general linear modelling (univariate analysis of variance). In addition to SNP genotype and sex, cohort was added to the model as a fixed effect and age 11 MHT score was included as a covariate.

All general linear modelling was performed using SPSS v12.0. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.01 for all statistical tests.

References

  1. Deary IJ, Whalley LJ, Lemmon H, Crawford JR, Starr JM: The stability of individual differences in mental ability from childhood to old age: follow-up of the 1932 Scottish Mental Survey. Intelligence. 2000, 28: 49-55. 10.1016/S0160-2896(99)00031-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Carroll JB: Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor Analytic Studies. 1993, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press

    Book  Google Scholar 

  3. Salthouse TA, Ferrer-Caja E: What needs to be explained to account for age-related effects on multiple cognitive variables?. Psychol Aging. 2003, 18: 91-110. 10.1037/0882-7974.18.1.91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Deary IJ, Spinath FM, Bates TC: Genetics of intelligence. Eur J Hum Genet. 2006, 14: 690-700. 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201588.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Deary IJ, Wright AF, Harris SE, Whalley LJ, Starr JM: Searching for genetic influences on normal cognitive ageing. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004, 8: 178-184. 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Petrill SA, Lipton PA, Hewitt JK, Plomin R, Cherny SS, Corley R, DeFries JC: Genetic and environmental contributions to general cognitive ability through the first 16 years of life. Dev Psychol. 2004, 40: 805-812. 10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.805.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Deary IJ, Whiteman MC, Pattie A, Starr JM, Hayward C, Wright AF, Carothers A, Whalley LJ: Cognitive change and the APOE epsilon 4 allele. Nature. 2002, 418: 932-10.1038/418932a.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Harman D: Aging: a theory based on free radical and radiation chemistry. J Gerontol. 1956, 11: 298-300.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Harman D: Role of free radicals in aging and disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1992, 673: 126-141. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb27444.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Balaban RS, Nemoto S, Finkel T: Mitochondria, oxidants, and aging. Cell. 2005, 120: 483-495. 10.1016/j.cell.2005.02.001.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Lombard DB, Chua KF, Mostoslavsky R, Franco S, Gostissa M, Alt FW: DNA repair, genome stability, and aging. Cell. 2005, 120: 497-512. 10.1016/j.cell.2005.01.028.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Halliwell B: Oxidative stress and neurodegeneration: where are we now?. J Neurochem. 2006, 97: 1634-1658. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.03907.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Mattson MP, Magnus T: Ageing and neuronal vulnerability. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006, 7: 278-294. 10.1038/nrn1886.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Kolosova NG, Shcheglova TV, Sergeeva SV, Loskutova LV: Long-term antioxidant supplementation attenuates oxidative stress markers and cognitive deficits in senescent-accelerated OXYS rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2006, 27: 1289-1297. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.07.022.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Chong ZZ, Li F, Maiese K: Oxidative stress in the brain: novel cellular targets that govern survival during neurodegenerative disease. Prog Neurobiol. 2005, 75: 207-246. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.02.004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Wallace DC: A mitochondrial paradigm of metabolic and degenerative diseases, aging, and cancer: a dawn for evolutionary medicine. Annu Rev Genet. 2005, 39: 359-407. 10.1146/annurev.genet.39.110304.095751.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Beal MF: Mitochondria take center stage in aging and neurodegeneration. Ann Neurol. 2005, 58: 495-505. 10.1002/ana.20624.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Lebovitz RM, Zhang H, Vogel H, Cartwright J, Dionne L, Lu N, Huang S, Matzuk MM: Neurodegeneration, myocardial injury, and perinatal death in mitochondrial superoxide dismutase-deficient mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996, 93: 9782-9787. 10.1073/pnas.93.18.9782.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Williams MD, Van Remmen H, Conrad CC, Huang TT, Epstein CJ, Richardson A: Increased oxidative damage is correlated to altered mitochondrial function in heterozygous manganese superoxide dismutase knockout mice. J Biol Chem. 1998, 273: 28510-28515. 10.1074/jbc.273.43.28510.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. al Chalabi A, Leigh PN: Recent advances in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Curr Opin Neurol. 2000, 13: 397-405. 10.1097/00019052-200008000-00006.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Hensley K, Butterfield DA, Hall N, Cole P, Subramaniam R, Mark R, Mattson MP, Markesbery WR, Harris ME, Aksenov M, .: Reactive oxygen species as causal agents in the neurotoxicity of the Alzheimer's disease-associated amyloid beta peptide. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1996, 786: 120-134. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1996.tb39057.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Chong ZZ, Li F, Maiese K: Stress in the brain: novel cellular mechanisms of injury linked to Alzheimer's disease. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2005, 49: 1-21. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.11.005.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Packer L: . Oxidative Stress and Aging. Edited by: Cutler RG. 1995, Basel, Birkhauser, 1-14.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  24. Keller JN, Schmitt FA, Scheff SW, Ding Q, Chen Q, Butterfield DA, Markesbery WR: Evidence of increased oxidative damage in subjects with mild cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2005, 64: 1152-1156.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Migliore L, Fontana I, Trippi F, Colognato R, Coppede F, Tognoni G, Nucciarone B, Siciliano G: Oxidative DNA damage in peripheral leukocytes of mild cognitive impairment and AD patients. Neurobiol Aging. 2005, 26: 567-573. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.07.016.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Berr C, Balansard B, Arnaud J, Roussel AM, Alperovitch A: Cognitive decline is associated with systemic oxidative stress: the EVA study. Etude du Vieillissement Arteriel. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000, 48: 1285-1291.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Anstey KJ, Dear K, Christensen H, Jorm AF: Biomarkers, health, lifestyle, and demographic variables as correlates of reaction time performance in early, middle, and late adulthood. Q J Exp Psychol A. 2005, 58: 5-21. 10.1080/02724980443000232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Lee CK, Weindruch R, Prolla TA: Gene-expression profile of the ageing brain in mice. Nat Genet. 2000, 25: 294-297. 10.1038/77046.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Jiang CH, Tsien JZ, Schultz PG, Hu Y: The effects of aging on gene expression in the hypothalamus and cortex of mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001, 98: 1930-1934. 10.1073/pnas.98.4.1930.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Kachiwala SJ, Harris SE, Wright AF, Hayward C, Starr JM, Whalley LJ, Deary IJ: Genetic influences on oxidative stress and their association with normal cognitive ageing. Neurosci Lett. 2005, 386: 116-120. 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.05.067.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Harris SE, Fox H, Wright AF, Hayward C, Starr JM, Whalley LJ, Deary IJ: The brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism is associated with age-related change in reasoning skills. Mol Psychiatry. 2006, 11: 505-513. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001799.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Deary IJ, Whiteman MC, Starr JM, Whalley LJ, Fox HC: The impact of childhood intelligence on later life: following up the Scottish mental surveys of 1932 and 1947. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004, 86: 130-147. 10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Deary IJ, Hamilton G, Hayward C, Whalley LJ, Powell J, Starr JM, Lovestone S: Nicastrin gene polymorphisms, cognitive ability level and cognitive ageing. Neurosci Lett. 2005, 373: 110-114. 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.09.073.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Selkoe DJ, Podlisny MB: Deciphering the genetic basis of Alzheimer's disease. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet. 2002, 3: 67-99. 10.1146/annurev.genom.3.022502.103022.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Lahiri DK, Ge YW, Maloney B, Wavrant-De Vrieze F, Hardy J: Characterization of two APP gene promoter polymorphisms that appear to influence risk of late-onset Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2005, 26: 1329-1341. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.11.005.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Koo EH, Sisodia SS, Cork LC, Unterbeck A, Bayney RM, Price DL: Differential expression of amyloid precursor protein mRNAs in cases of Alzheimer's disease and in aged nonhuman primates. Neuron. 1990, 4: 97-104. 10.1016/0896-6273(90)90446-M.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Palmert MR, Golde TE, Cohen ML, Kovacs DM, Tanzi RE, Gusella JF, Usiak MF, Younkin LH, Younkin SG: Amyloid protein precursor messenger RNAs: differential expression in Alzheimer's disease. Science. 1988, 241: 1080-1084. 10.1126/science.2457949.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Reddy PH: Amyloid precursor protein-mediated free radicals and oxidative damage: implications for the development and progression of Alzheimer's disease. J Neurochem. 2006, 96: 1-13. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2005.03530.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Promoter 2.0 Prediction Server. 2007, [http://www.cbs.dtu.dk/services/Promoter/]

  40. Human - UCSC Genome Browser v161. 2007, [http://genome.ucsc.edu/cgi-bin/hgTracks]

  41. Thomas DC, Haile RW, Duggan D: Recent developments in genomewide association scans: a workshop summary and review. Am J Hum Genet. 2005, 77: 337-345. 10.1086/432962.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Gonzalez-Neira A, Ke X, Lao O, Calafell F, Navarro A, Comas D, Cann H, Bumpstead S, Ghori J, Hunt S, Deloukas P, Dunham I, Cardon LR, Bertranpetit J: The portability of tagSNPs across populations: a worldwide survey. Genome Res. 2006, 16: 323-330. 10.1101/gr.4138406.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Tenesa A, Dunlop MG: Validity of tagging SNPs across populations for association studies. Eur J Hum Genet. 2006, 14: 357-363. 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201554.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Montpetit A, Nelis M, Laflamme P, Magi R, Ke X, Remm M, Cardon L, Hudson TJ, Metspalu A: An evaluation of the performance of tag SNPs derived from HapMap in a Caucasian population. PLoS Genet. 2006, 2: e27-10.1371/journal.pgen.0020027.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Terwilliger JD, Hiekkalinna T: An utter refutation of the "Fundamental Theorem of the HapMap". Eur J Hum Genet. 2006, 14: 426-437. 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201583.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Marchini J, Donnelly P, Cardon LR: Genome-wide strategies for detecting multiple loci that influence complex diseases. Nat Genet. 2005, 37: 413-417. 10.1038/ng1537.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Scottish Council for Research in Education: The intelligence of Scottish children: A national survey of an age-group. 1933, London, University of London Press

    Google Scholar 

  48. Scottish Council for Research in Education: The trend of Scottish intelligence: A comparison of the 1947 and 1932 Surveys of the intelligence of eleven-year-old pupils. 1949, London, University of London Press

    Google Scholar 

  49. Whalley LJ, Fox HC, Starr JM, Deary IJ: Age at natural menopause and cognition. Maturitas. 2004, 49: 148-156. 10.1016/j.maturitas.2003.12.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Whalley LJ, Fox HC, Deary IJ, Starr JM: Childhood IQ, smoking, and cognitive change from age 11 to 64 years. Addictive Behaviors. 2005, 30: 77-88. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.04.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Wechsler D: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. 1981, New York, Psychological Corporation

    Google Scholar 

  52. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975, 12: 189-198. 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Deary IJ, Leaper SA, Murray AD, Staff RT, Whalley LJ: Cerebral white matter abnormalities and lifetime cognitive change: a 67-year follow-up of the Scottish Mental Survey of 1932. Psychol Aging. 2003, 18: 140-148. 10.1037/0882-7974.18.1.140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Raven JC, Court JH, Raven J: Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. 1977, London, H. K. Lewis

    Google Scholar 

  55. Lezak M: Neuropsychological testing. 1995, Oxford, England, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  56. Boone KB, Ponton MO, Gorsuch RL, Gonzalez JJ, Miller BL: Factor analysis of four measures of prefrontal lobe functioning. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1998, 13: 585-595. 10.1016/S0887-6177(97)00074-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Wechsler D: Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. 1987, New York, Psychological Corporation

    Google Scholar 

  58. Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Facility Edinburgh. 2007, [http://www.wtcrf.ed.ac.uk]

  59. Purcell S, Cherny SS, Sham PC: Genetic Power Calculator: design of linkage and association genetic mapping studies of complex traits. Bioinformatics. 2003, 19: 149-150. 10.1093/bioinformatics/19.1.149.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Comings DE, Wu S, Rostamkhani M, McGue M, Lacono WG, Cheng LS, MacMurray JP: Role of the cholinergic muscarinic 2 receptor (CHRM2) gene in cognition. Mol Psychiatry. 2003, 8: 10-11. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001095.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Papassotiropoulos A, Bagli M, Feder O, Jessen F, Maier W, Rao ML, Ludwig M, Schwab SG, Heun R: Genetic polymorphism of cathepsin D is strongly associated with the risk for developing sporadic Alzheimer's disease. Neurosci Lett. 1999, 262: 171-174. 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)00071-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Payton A, Holland F, Diggle P, Rabbitt P, Horan M, Davidson Y, Gibbons L, Worthington J, Ollier WE, Pendleton N: Cathepsin D exon 2 polymorphism associated with general intelligence in a healthy older population. Mol Psychiatry. 2003, 8: 14-18. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001239.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. de Quervain DJ, Henke K, Aerni A, Coluccia D, Wollmer MA, Hock C, Nitsch RM, Papassotiropoulos A: A functional genetic variation of the 5-HT2a receptor affects human memory. Nat Neurosci. 2003, 6: 1141-1142. 10.1038/nn1146.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Lam LC, Tang NL, Ma SL, Zhang W, Chiu HF: 5-HT2A T102C receptor polymorphism and neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer's disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2004, 19: 523-526. 10.1002/gps.1109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Edland SD, Wavrant-De Vriese F, Compton D, Smith GE, Ivnik R, Boeve BF, Tangalos EG, Petersen RC: Insulin degrading enzyme (IDE) genetic variants and risk of Alzheimer's disease: evidence of effect modification by apolipoprotein E (APOE). Neurosci Lett. 2003, 345: 21-24. 10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00488-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Sciacca FL, Ferri C, Licastro F, Veglia F, Biunno I, Gavazzi A, Calabrese E, Martinelli BF, Sorbi S, Mariani C, Franceschi M, Grimaldi LM: Interleukin-1B polymorphism is associated with age at onset of Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2003, 24: 927-931. 10.1016/S0197-4580(03)00011-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Sole-Padulles C, Bartres-Faz D, Junque C, Via M, Matarin M, Gonzalez-Perez E, Moral P, Moya A, Clemente IC: Poorer cognitive performance in humans with mild cognitive impairment carrying the T variant of the Glu/Asp NOS3 polymorphism. Neurosci Lett. 2004, 358: 5-8. 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.12.044.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Ertekin-Taner N, Ronald J, Feuk L, Prince J, Tucker M, Younkin L, Hella M, Jain S, Hackett A, Scanlin L, Kelly J, Kihiko-Ehman M, Neltner M, Hersh L, Kindy M, Markesbery W, Hutton M, de Andrade M, Petersen RC, Graff-Radford N, Estus S, Brookes AJ, Younkin SG: Elevated amyloid beta protein (Abeta42) and late onset Alzheimer's disease are associated with single nucleotide polymorphisms in the urokinase-type plasminogen activator gene. Hum Mol Genet. 2005, 14: 447-460. 10.1093/hmg/ddi041.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Forsberg L, Lyrenas L, de Faire U, Morgenstern R: A common functional C-T substitution polymorphism in the promoter region of the human catalase gene influences transcription factor binding, reporter gene transcription and is correlated to blood catalase levels. Free Radic Biol Med. 2001, 30: 500-505. 10.1016/S0891-5849(00)00487-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Butcher LM, Meaburn E, Dale PS, Sham P, Schalkwyk LC, Craig IW, Plomin R: Association analysis of mild mental impairment using DNA pooling to screen 432 brain-expressed single-nucleotide polymorphisms. Mol Psychiatry. 2005, 10: 384-392. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001589.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Yaffe K, Edwards ER, Lui LY, Zmuda JM, Ferrell RE, Cauley JA: Androgen receptor CAG repeat polymorphism is associated with cognitive function in older men. Biol Psychiatry. 2003, 54: 943-946. 10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00115-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Cao L, Jiao X, Zuzga DS, Liu Y, Fong DM, Young D, During MJ: VEGF links hippocampal activity with neurogenesis, learning and memory. Nat Genet. 2004, 36: 827-835. 10.1038/ng1395.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Yan SD, Chen X, Fu J, Chen M, Zhu H, Roher A, Slattery T, Zhao L, Nagashima M, Morser J, Migheli A, Nawroth P, Stern D, Schmidt AM: RAGE and amyloid-beta peptide neurotoxicity in Alzheimer's disease. Nature. 1996, 382: 685-691. 10.1038/382685a0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Reynolds CA, Jansson M, Gatz M, Pedersen NL: Longitudinal change in memory performance associated with HTR2A polymorphism. Neurobiol Aging. 2006, 27: 150-154. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.12.009.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Martha Whiteman and Alison Pattie who collected phenotype data on the LBC1921 subjects. We thank Jen Herbert and others who collected phenotype data on the ABC1936 subjects. This project was funded by the United Kingdom Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council. Ian Deary holds a Royal Society-Wolfson Research Merit award. Lawrence Whalley was supported by a Wellcome Trust Career Development Award.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ian J Deary.

Additional information

Authors' contributions

IJD, AFW, LJW and JMS originally designed the study. All authors contributed to the development of the study design and helped draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript. SEH curated the LBC1921 database, performed the statistical analysis and drafted the manuscript. HF curated the ABC1936 database.

Electronic supplementary material

12863_2007_522_MOESM1_ESM.doc

Additional file 1: 384 SNPs selected for genotyping by Illumina. The table provided lists the 384 SNPs that were submitted to Illumina for genotyping. Predicted SNP function, amino acid substitution (where relevant), percentage identity in mouse, and gene and chromosome locations are given for each SNP. (DOC 511 KB)

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Harris, S.E., Fox, H., Wright, A.F. et al. A genetic association analysis of cognitive ability and cognitive ageing using 325 markers for 109 genes associated with oxidative stress or cognition. BMC Genet 8, 43 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2156-8-43

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2156-8-43

Keywords